A Relação entre Extremismo Político, Ilusão de Conhecimento e Crenças Conspiratórias e seus Impactos nos Eleitores de Três Municípios do Rio Grande do Sul
Resumo
As pessoas tipicamente sabem menos sobre política do que pensam. Quando se trata de extremistas, essa autoilusão é ainda maior. Além disso, os extremistas são mais suscetíveis a crenças de que diversos problemas socioeconômicos têm origem em conspirações, dificultando a implementação de estratégias formais de marketing eleitoral e de governo. Uma survey com amostra probabilística de 380 eleitores contendo questões que mensuraram o nível de extremismo e conhecimento sobre determinadas políticas públicas e a crença em teorias conspiratórias foi aplicada em três cidades gaúchas. Encontrou-se uma relação positiva entre posições mais extremas e o nível autodeclarado de conhecimento sobre diversos temas, além de uma associação positiva entre extremismo e crenças conspiratórias. Uma vez que os extremistas são mais convictos de seus conhecimentos, é possível que as pessoas com opiniões equilibradas sejam, constantemente, influenciadas pelas decisões tomadas por aquele grupo, muitas vezes lastradas por crenças improváveis.
Palavras-chave
Referências
Akerlof, G. A., & Shiller, R. J. (2015). Phishing for phools: The economics of manipulation and deception. Princeton, NJ, US: Princeton University Press.
Bird, S. T., & Bogart, L. M. (2003). Conspiracy beliefs about HIV/AIDS and birth control among African Americans: Implications for the prevention of HIV, other STIs, and unintended pregnancy. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 109–126.
Canetti, Daphna, & Miriam Lindner. (2015). Exposure to Political Violence and Political Behavior, Psychology of Change: Life Contexts, Experiences, and Identities, ed. Katherine Reynolds and Nyla Branscombe. New York: Psychology Press, 77–95.
Canetti-Nisim, Daphna, Eran Halperin, Keren Sharvit, & Stevan E Hobfoll. (2009). A new stress-based model of political extremism personal exposure to terrorism, psychological distress, and exclusionist political attitudes. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 53 (3), 363–389.
Dagnall N., Drinkwater K., Parker A., Denovan A., & Porton M. (2015). “Conspiracy theory and cognitive style: a worldview”. Frontiers in Psychology.
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influenced by personal willingness to conspire. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50, 542-552.
Downs, A. (1957). An Economic Theory of Political Action in a Democracy. Journal of Political Economy, 65, (2), 135-50.
Dropp, K. A., Kertzer, J. D., & Zeitzoff, T. (2014). The Less Americans Know about Ukraine’s Location, the More They Want U.S. to Intervene. Washington Post.
Fernbach, P. M., Rogers, T., Fox, C. R., & Sloman, S. A. (2013). Political extremism is supported by an illusion of understanding. Psychological Science, 24, 939–946.
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A Very Brief Measure of the Big Five Personality Domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504-528.
Greenberg, J., & Jonas, E. (2003). Psychological motives and political orientation—The left, the right, and the rigid: Comment on Jost. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 376-382.
Grosjean, Pauline. (2014). Conflict and social and political preferences: Evidence from World War II and civil conflict in 35 European countries. Comparative Economic Studies, 56(3), 424–451.
Hall, C. C., Ariss, L., & Todorov, A. (2007). The illusion of knowledge: When more information reduces accuracy and increases confidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103(2), 277-290.
Hardin,R. (2002). The crippled epistemology of extremism. In A. Breton, G. Galeotti, P. Salmon, & R. Wintrobe (Eds.), Political extremism and rationality, 3-22.
Heath, C., & Tversky, A. (1991). Preference and belief: ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 4(1), 5-28.
Hofstadter, R. (1966). The paranoid style in American politics. In R. Hofstadter (Ed.), The paranoid style in American politics and other essays. New York, NY: Knopf.
Ivereigh, A. (2006). The Da Vinci Code. Retrieved on November 2, 2010. Available from http://www.rcdow.org.uk/davincicode/
Kardon, B. E. (1992). Consumer schizophrenia: Extremism in the marketplace, Planning Review, 20(4), 18-22.
Kline, P. (2000). Handbook of Psychological Testing. London: Routledge.
McHoskey, J. W. (1995). Case closed? On the John F. Kennedy assassination: Biased assimilation of evidence and attitude polarization. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 395-409.
Miguel, E., Saiegh, S. M., & Satyanath, S. (2011). Civil War Exposure and Violence. Economics & Politics, 23(1), 59–73. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0343.2010.00372.x
Newheiser, A., Farias, M., & Tausch, N. 2011. The Functional Nature of Conspiracy Beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(8), 1007-11.
Robins, R. S., & Post, J. M. (1997). Political paranoia: The psychopolitics of hatred. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Rozenblit, L., & Keil, F. C. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science, 26, 521–562.
Schickel, R. (2005). Clint Eastwood on “Baby.” TimeMagazine. Retrieved from http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1029865,00.html
Schkade, D., Sunstein, C. R., & Hastie, R. (2010). When deliberation produces extremism. Critical Review: A Journal of Politics and Society, 22, 227–252.
Sharp, D. (2008). Advances in conspiracy theory. The Lancet, 372(9647), 1371–1372.
Shermer, M. (1997). Why people believe weird things: Pseudoscience, superstition, and other confusions of our time. New York: Freeman.
Sloman, S., & Fernbach, P. (2017). The Knowledge Illusion. New York: Riverhead Books.
Soni, D. (2007). Survey: ‘Government Hasn’t Told Truth about 7/7’. Channel 4 News. Available at: http://www.channel4.com
Sutton, R. M., & Douglas, K. M. (2014). Examining the monological nature of conspiracy theories in Power, Politics, and Paranoia, eds J. W. van Prooijen, and P. A. M. van Lange. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 254–272. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139565417.018
Tetlock, P. E., Armor, D., Peterson, R. S. (1994). The slavery debate in antebellum America: Cognitive style, value conflict, and the limits of compromise. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 115-126.
Thorburn, S., & Bogart, L. M. (2005). Conspiracy beliefs about birth control: Barriers to pregnancy prevention among African Americans of reproductive age. Health Education and Behavior, 32, 474-487.
Toner, K., Leary, M., Asher, M. W., & Jongman-Sereno, K. P. (2013). Feeling superior is a bipartisan issue: Extremity (not direction) of political views predicts perceived belief superiority. Psychological Science, 24, 2454–2462.
Uscinski, J. E., Parent, J. M., & Torres, B. (2011). Conspiracy theories are for losers, in Paper Presented at the 2011 American Political Science Association Annual Conference, Seattle, WA
Van, J-W. P., Krouwel, A. P. M., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Political Extremism Predicts Belief in Conspiracy Theories, Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 570-578.
Wood, S. A., & Hampson, S. E. (2005). Measuring the Big Five with single items using a bipolar response scale. European Journal of Personality, 19, 373-390.
Zonis, M., & Joseph, C. G. (1994). Conspiracy thinking in the Middle East. Political Psychology, 15, 443-459.
DOI: https://doi.org/10.18256/2237-7956.2018.v8i2.2534
Apontamentos
- Não há apontamentos.
Direitos autorais 2018 George dos Reis Alba, Andriele Nahara, Cleiton Arnhold, Alexandre Kieling, Gilmar D'Agostini Oliveira Casalinho
Revista de Administração IMED (RAIMED) ISSN: 2237-7956 Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração (PPGA/Atitus)
Atitus Educação - Business School – www.imed.edu.br – Rua Senador Pinheiro, 304 – Bairro Vila Rodrigues – 99070-220 – Passo Fundo/RS – Brasil Tel.: +55 51 4004-4818
Esta obra está licenciada com uma Licença Creative Commons Atribuição 4.0 Internacional.